Accumulating Evidence Suggests That Men Do Not Find Body Odours of Human Leucocyte Antigen-Dissimilar Women More Attractive
PROCEEDINGS OF THE ROYAL SOCIETY B-BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES(2018)
摘要
You have accessMoreSectionsView PDF ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinked InRedditEmail Cite this article Lobmaier Janek S., Fischbacher Urs, Probst Fabian, Wirthmüller Urs and Knoch Daria 2018Accumulating evidence suggests that men do not find body odours of human leucocyte antigen-dissimilar women more attractiveProc. R. Soc. B.2852018056620180566http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0566SectionYou have accessInvited replyAccumulating evidence suggests that men do not find body odours of human leucocyte antigen-dissimilar women more attractive Janek S. Lobmaier Janek S. Lobmaier http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8025-1616 Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland [email protected] Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author , Urs Fischbacher Urs Fischbacher Department of Economics, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany Thurgau Institute of Economics, Kreuzlingen, Switzerland Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author , Fabian Probst Fabian Probst University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author , Urs Wirthmüller Urs Wirthmüller Universitätsinstitut für Klinische Chemie, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author and Daria Knoch Daria Knoch http://orcid.org/0000-0003-1935-053X Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland [email protected] Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Search for more papers by this author Janek S. Lobmaier Janek S. Lobmaier http://orcid.org/0000-0001-8025-1616 Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland [email protected] Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed , Urs Fischbacher Urs Fischbacher Department of Economics, University of Konstanz, Konstanz, Germany Thurgau Institute of Economics, Kreuzlingen, Switzerland Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed , Fabian Probst Fabian Probst University Hospital of Psychiatry Zurich, University of Zurich, Zurich, Switzerland Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed , Urs Wirthmüller Urs Wirthmüller Universitätsinstitut für Klinische Chemie, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed and Daria Knoch Daria Knoch http://orcid.org/0000-0003-1935-053X Institute of Psychology, University of Bern, Bern, Switzerland [email protected] Google Scholar Find this author on PubMed Published:09 May 2018https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0566In our recent study published in Proceedings of the Royal Society B [1], we investigated the effects of major histocompatibility complex (MHC; or human leucocyte antigen system, HLA in humans) on men's preferences for women's body odours. Using rigorous methods, we found no evidence that men find body odours of HLA-dissimilar women more attractive than odours of HLA-similar women. In his comment, Wedekind [2] claims that our conclusions were premature, because he found (using our data) a stronger negative relationship between pleasantness and intensity for HLA-dissimilar odours than for similar ones. Although this is an interesting finding, it cannot be considered as a support for HLA-related mate preferences.Wedekind's comment is rather misleading, for various reasons. First, empirical evidence for HLA-mediated body odour preferences in humans is not nearly as clear as the author would like to assume. In the 20 years since Wedekind et al.'s seminal work [3], various studies examining HLA-influences on body odour preferences in humans have been published and the reported findings are mixed. In fact, a recent meta-analysis by Winternitz, Abbate [4] revealed that over all studies with human and non-human primates, results on MHC-mediated body odour preferences are inconsistent and non-significant. It is important to note that Wedekind [2] misinterprets the meta-analysis by Winternitz et al. [4]: These authors found preferences for MHC heterozygosity but not for MHC dissimilarity. These are two very different kinds of MHC influences and should not be mixed up. Another recent review outlining disparate and controversial results for human studies is Winternitz & Abbate [5].Second, Wedekind compares our study with two of his own studies [3,6]. However, our study differs from Wedekind's studies in at least one important aspect: we studied men's preferences for women's body odours while Wedekind et al. [3] asked women to evaluate the body odours of men. Wedekind & Furi [6] also included odours of very few women (i.e. two), which may have led to a false positive result. There is reason to believe that males and females might differ regarding the effect of MHC in mate preferences (cf. [6–9]). Because males typically have a higher potential reproductive rate [11] and females often bear greater reproductive costs [12], males might seek females with high perceived fertility to increase the probability for reproduction, while females seek males who are most likely to maximize offspring survival. So, it is conceivable that men rely less on HLA-mediated odour cues when choosing their mates than women. We note that apart from our study [1], only four studies have included men as odour raters. Two of these studies report no preference HLA-dissimilar women [13,14], and two suggest that men show a preference for body odours of HLA-dissimilar women [6,15] (note that Wedekind & Furi included odours of only two women). These four studies have in common that they did not control for the menstrual cycle phase during which the body odours were collected. Controlling for menstrual cycle phase is important because body odour varies across the cycle. An increasing number of studies report that women's body odour is rated as more attractive if gathered near ovulation compared to odour that was collected in other cycle phases [16–19]. So, if not controlling for menstrual cycle, cyclic effects of odour attractiveness are likely to overwrite any putative HLA-effects. In our recent study [1], we controlled for menstrual cycle phase. We also included a large number of female odour donors (n = 49) and collected ratings of 94 men. The results of this study add to the studies suggesting no effect of HLA similarity on women's preferences for men's body odours.Furthermore, Wedekind mentions in his comment [2] that the female donors in our study were asked to shave their armpits 2–5 days before odour collection, which in his view might reduce the potential link between HLA and body odour attractiveness. However, such a link (if it exists) should still be observable in our study, because only body odours that were perceivable were included in the analyses. We note that only in very few cases (less than 5%) odours were rated as 'non-perceivable'. In addition, it is important to note that the non-perceivable trials were not always from the same pad (i.e. woman). In other words, there was no pad that was not perceivable in all cases: the non-perceivable pads did not come from specific women, but were randomly distributed over different donors. Since men clearly found some odours more attractive than others, it seems unlikely that the odours were too faint to be smelled. It is not surprising that in Wedekind's original study no single odour was rated as 'not perceivable', because in his study, it seems that raters were not given the option to choose 'I cannot perceive the odour'. Wedekind further mentions that the odours that could be perceived were on average rated as less intense in our study [1] than in Wedekind's study that allowed for axillary hairs [3]. Again, this is not surprising, as Probst et al. [1] included only women as odour donors while Wedekind et al. [3] included odours of only men, and men's body odours are typically perceived as being more intense than women's (e.g. [20,21]).The finding that the negative correlation between intensity and pleasantness ratings was stronger for HLA dissimilar than for similar odours is potentially interesting, but the reasons for this interaction is currently unclear. Moreover, the negative relationship between intensity and pleasantness was present in both HLA similar and dissimilar odours and the interaction was only weak. The main interest of Probst et al.'s [1] study was to investigate potential HLA-mediated odour preferences in men. We found that none of HLA similarity, HLA heterozygosity, nor rareness of HLA had an effect on how attractive men found women's body odours. The reanalyses by Wedekind do not contradict these findings.To conclude, we find Wedekind's comment [2] on our recent study [1] is based on many questionable assumptions which, when corrected and put into the right context, do not make a convincing point in challenging our main claims. While the interaction between odour pleasantness and intensity reported in Wedekind's comment might indeed point towards the existence of some form of social signalling, it cannot be considered as a support for HLA-mediated mate preferences.Data accessibilityThe dataset used in our original work is available at Dryad Digital Repository: (http://dx.doi.org/10.5061/dryad.270h8).Authors' contributionsJ.S.L., U.F., F.P. and D.K. provided helpful inputs in various discussions; J.S.L. and D.K. wrote the manuscript; U.F., F.P. and U.W. provided helpful input on manuscript drafts.Competing interestsWe declare we have no competing interests.FundingWe received no funding for this study.FootnotesThe accompanying comment can be viewed at http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.2714.© 2018 The Author(s)Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved.References1Probst F, Fischbacher U, Lobmaier JS, Wirthmuller U, Knoch D. 2017Men's preferences for women's body odours are not associated with human leucocyte antigen. Proc. R. Soc. B 284, 20171830. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.1830) Link, ISI, Google Scholar2Wedekind C. 2018A predicted interaction between odour pleasantness and intensity provides evidence for major histocompatibility complex social signalling in women. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20172714. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2017.2714) Link, ISI, Google Scholar3Wedekind C, Seebeck T, Bettens F, Paepke AJ. 1995MHC-dependent mate preferences in humans. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 260, 245–249. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1995.0087) Link, ISI, Google Scholar4Winternitz J, Abbate JL, Huchard E, Havlicek J, Garamszegi LZ. 2017Patterns of MHC-dependent mate selection in humans and nonhuman primates: a meta-analysis. Mol. Ecol. 26, 668–688. (doi:10.1111/mec.13920) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar5Winternitz J, Abbate JL. 2015Examining the evidence for major histocompatibility complex-dependent mate selection in humans and nonhuman primates. Res. Rep. Biol. 6, 73–88. (doi:10.2147/RRB.S58514) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar6Wedekind C, Furi S. 1997Body odour preferences in men and women: do they aim for specific MHC combinations or simply heterozygosity?Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 264, 1471–1479. (doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0204) Link, ISI, Google Scholar7Neff BD, Garner SR, Heath JW, Heath DD. 2008The MHC and non-random mating in a captive population of Chinook salmon. Heredity 101, 175–185. (doi:10.1038/hdy.2008.43) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar8Egid K, Brown JL. 1989The major histocompatibility complex and female mating preferences in mice. Anim. Behav. 38, 548–550. (doi:10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80051-X) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar9Eklund A. 1997The major histocompatibility complex and mating preferences in wild house mice (Mus domesticus). Behav. Ecol. 8, 630–634. (doi:10.1093/beheco/8.6.630) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar10Huchard E, Knapp LA, Wang J, Raymond M, Cowlishaw G. 2010MHC, mate choice and heterozygote advantage in a wild social primate. Mol. Ecol. 19, 2545–2561. (doi:10.1111/j.1365-294X.2010.04644.x) PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar11Clutton-Brock TH, Parker GA. 1992Potential reproductive rates and the operation of sexual selection. Q. Rev. Biol. 67, 437–456. (doi:10.1086/417793) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar12Trivers R. 1972Parental investment and sexual selection. In Sexual selection and the descent of man (ed. Campbell B), pp. 136–179. Chicago, IL: Aldine-Atherton. Google Scholar13Kromer J, Hummel T, Pietrowski D, Giani AS, Sauter J, Ehninger G, Schmidt AH, Croy I. 2016Influence of HLA on human partnership and sexual satisfaction. Sci. Rep. 6, 32550. (doi:10.1038/srep32550) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar14Santos PSC, Schinemann JA, Gabardo J, Bicalho MD. 2005New evidence that the MHC influences odor perception in humans: a study with 58 Southern Brazilian students. Horm. Behav. 47, 384–388. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.11.005) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar15Thornhill R, Gangestad SW, Miller R, Scheyd G, McCollough JK, Franklin M. 2003Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and women. Behav. Ecol. 14, 668–678. (doi:10.1093/beheco/arg043) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar16Cerda-Molina AL, Hernandez-Lopez L, de la O CE, Chavira-Ramirez R, Mondragon-Ceballos R. 2013Changes in men's salivary testosterone and cortisol levels, and in sexual desire after smelling female axillary and vulvar scents. Front. Endocrinol. 4, 159. (doi:10.3389/fendo.2013.00159) Crossref, PubMed, Google Scholar17Gildersleeve KA, Haselton MG, Larson CM, Pillsworth EG. 2012Body odor attractiveness as a cue of impending ovulation in women: evidence from a study using hormone-confirmed ovulation. Horm. Behav. 61, 157–166. (doi:10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.11.005) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar18Havlicek J, Dvorakova R, Bartos L, Flegr J. 2006Non-advertised does not mean concealed: body odour changes across the human menstrual cycle. Ethology 112, 81–90. (doi:10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01125.x) Crossref, ISI, Google Scholar19Singh D, Bronstad PM. 2001Female body odour is a potential cue to ovulation. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 268, 797–801. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2001.1589) Link, ISI, Google Scholar20Doty RL, Orndorff MM, Leyden J, Kligman A. 1978Communication of gender from human axillary odors: relationship to perceived intensity and hedonicity. Behav. Biol. 23, 373–380. (doi:10.1016/S0091-6773(78)91393-7) Crossref, PubMed, Google Scholar21Chen D, Haviland-Jones J. 1999Rapid mood change and human odors. Physiol. Behav. 68, 241–250. (doi:10.1016/S0031-9384(99)00147-X) Crossref, PubMed, ISI, Google Scholar Previous ArticleNext Article VIEW FULL TEXT DOWNLOAD PDF FiguresRelatedReferencesDetailsCited byWyatt T (2020) Reproducible research into human chemical communication by cues and pheromones: learning from psychology's renaissance, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375:1800, Online publication date: 8-Jun-2020. Sorokowska A, Pietrowski D, Schäfer L, Kromer J, Schmidt A, Sauter J, Hummel T and Croy I (2018) Human Leukocyte Antigen similarity decreases partners' and strangers' body odor attractiveness for women not using hormonal contraception, Hormones and Behavior, 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2018.10.007, 106, (144-149), Online publication date: 1-Nov-2018. Yamaguchi and Dijkstra (2019) Major Histocompatibility Complex (MHC) Genes and Disease Resistance in Fish, Cells, 10.3390/cells8040378, 8:4, (378) This Issue16 May 2018Volume 285Issue 1878 Article InformationDOI:https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.0566PubMed:29743256Published by:Royal SocietyOnline ISSN:1471-2954History: Manuscript received13/03/2018Manuscript accepted11/04/2018Published online09/05/2018Published in print16/05/2018 License:© 2018 The Author(s)Published by the Royal Society. All rights reserved. Citations and impact Subjectsbehaviourevolutiongenetics Large datasets are available through Proceedings B's partnership with Dryad
更多查看译文
关键词
House Dust Mite Allergens
AI 理解论文
溯源树
样例
生成溯源树,研究论文发展脉络
Chat Paper
正在生成论文摘要